Here at Retraction Watch, we’ve been following the case of Jatinder Ahluwalia with interest. You may recall that an investigation by University College London (UCL) found “beyond reasonable doubt” that Ahluwalia had renumbered files to deceive a co-author. UCL was also “highly confident” that Ahluwalia had messed with his solutions to make his results look better, and sabotaged his colleagues’ work. The report of that investigation was part of a Nature retraction notice.
We’ve now learned that UCL was not the first scene of misconduct by Ahluwalia. Yesterday, we obtained letters by University of Cambridge faculty and administrators describing repeated — and in the words of of one professor, amateurish — data fabrication by Ahluwalia that led to his dismissal from the university’s graduate program.
In a letter dated November 10, 1997, Martin Brand, then Ahluwalia’s PhD advisor, wrote:
…I am no longer prepared to act as PhD supervisor for Jatinder Ahluwalia, and…recommend that he removed from the Board’s list of graduate students because I believe he has been inventing experimental results.
Ahluwalia joined Brand’s lab in August 1996, Brand explained in the letter to the secretary of Cambridge’s board of graduate studies:
Various members of my research group were involved in showing him how to carry out his project, with my guidance. During this time we had many discussions about his work, and he presented results in our research group lab seminars and showed me examples of his raw data. I specifically instructed him how to keep adequate records of his experiments.
Ahluwalia’s certificate of postgraduate studies — CPGS, a prerequisite for a PhD — was approved in October 1997. But there were already questions:
On 14th October 1997 I recommended (expressing some reservations) that he be registered for a PhD. These reservations were based on a loss of confidence in the data that he used for the CPGS.
Those doubts had surfaced in the summer, when Brand was carrying out and lecturing in Australia but in touch with Ahluwalia about some discrepancies in the data.
When I returned we spent 3 days attempting to go through his experimental results together (19, 22, 23 September 1997). At that stage it became clear that his documentation of what he had done was at best totally inadequate and at worst very suspicious. I had told him to have all of his data to hand before our discussions, but despite sending him back to his room in Cherry Hinton three times on the same day to collect missing traces and data, he could not satisfactorily match data to his graphs. Because of the difficulty in working out what had been done, we were only able to go through a small part of his data in detail. For example, he had graphs in his CPGS thesis relating to experiments involving the use of rats and radioactivity, but examination of our animal house and radioactive use and disposal records showed no entries for the dates he claimed to have done the experiments (such records are a legal requirement that we take seriously). He also initially claimed to have measured the dry weights of each of his cell preparations, but on closer questioning it transpired that he had simply assumed these values. I pointed out that it looked possible that he had been falsifying data and that this was a very serious matter that could not be tolerated. However, I was prepared to give him the benefit of the doubt and to put the problem down to incompetence and poor record-keeping. I asked him to go away and work out from his lab book records exactly what he had done on every experimental day since he started, and to produce a summary log of experiments and results that we could then use as a basis for further discussions.
Brand and Ahluwalia did experiments together from September 26 through October 9, “to establish reliable values for one of his experiments, and to teach him how to carry out the experiments correctly.” When they went through the log of experiments on November 6:
…close inspection of some of the experimental traces convinced me that they had been falsified, perhaps to provide the data I was demanding to support the tables in his CPGS thesis.
At this point, best to let the letter speak for itself:
Specifically, the experimental traces produced at this meeting by Jat and dated 9/12/96 and 11/12/96 were y-t strip chart recorder traces that display oxygen concentration in a cuvette as a function of time. The recorder moves the paper at a smooth fixed rate (giving the x-axis) underneath a pen that moves according to oxygen concentration on the y-axis. Although the speed can be changed to vary the scale of the x-axis, it cannot be set on our recorders to make time a variable, so essentially all the experimental noise must appear as pen movements in the y-axis. I have used this type of apparatus continually since 1971, and know its characteristics well. When additions are made to the experimental cuvette, spikes can sometimes occur on the oxygen (y) axis, but they do not occur on the time (x) axis.
The traces I was shown by Jat show large spikes along the time axis at points representing additions to the cuvette. They also show smaller noise which runs along the time axis. For a recorder to produce such spikes and noise the paper would first have to come out of the recorder, then go back in, many times during an experimental run. This has never happened in my experience, and the recorder used by Jat for these experiments has remained in use in the lab since December 1996 and has not been found to be faulty.
These traces show other anomalies too; for example they are on two different numbered rolls of paper (B9 and C9) and Jat told me that they were produced in the order B9, C9, B9, C9, yet there was no reason to have gone through the clumsy procedure of changing the paper or chart recorder between runs and Jat has not claimed that he did so. I also asked the senior postdoc in my lab, Dr David Rolfe, to look at the traces, without saying what I thought was wrong with them, and he independently made the same assessment as me – that they could not have been produced on our apparatus in the way that Jat maintains they were.
Jat has no explanation for these spikes on his traces. Indeed, I’m not sure that he understands why they show so clearly that the traces have been falsified. I offered him the opportunity to show the evidence to Professor Blundell, the Head of the Biochemistry Department, if he was in any way dissatisfied with my interpretation of the situation, but he declined. I then told him that if he could not come up with a satisfactory explanation of the genesis of the traces, I would not allow him to continue in my lab, and I would not recommend to any other supervisor that he be allowed to work on some other project. He did not offer any convincing explanation. I have since written to him repeating the offer of an opportunity to demonstrate that he can produce such experimental traces on the same equipment before witnesses if he wishes.
Based on this evidence, Brand wrote, he could not “see reasonable doubt that they were falsified.”
Under the circumstances I am not prepared to continue to supervise Jatinder Ahluwalia’s PhD studies and I recommend to the Board that they strike his name off the register of Graduate students.
Brand sent Ahluwalia a copy of his letter, and offered again to let him repeat his experiments with witnesses. Ahluwalia evidently didn’t take advantage of that offer. He lost his studentship funding from the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council at the end of 1997, and was dismissed from the graduate studies program on February 18, 1998.
This week, Brand — now working at the Buck Institute for Research on Aging in San Francisco — said “it seems that [Ahluwalia] did not learn from his experience.”
We agree.
Perhaps it is understandable that Imperial College, where Ahluwalia eventually earned his PhD, gave him a second chance. But he had a second strike at UCL, and yet he still has a position at the University of East London — where, we’ve noted, plagiarism is one of his areas of study.
We tried to reach Ahluwalia for comment, as we have for our previous posts. He has never gotten back to us, but we will update if he does.
One could ask the Dean if they were aware of his track record. And if so, why they hired him.
Even though the Anil Potti case concerns resume padding, we can still make a similar comparison here. Duke maintains that it shouldn’t have to background check every award or credential of its faculty recruits, but takes the applicants’ resumes on face value (the benefit of the doubt). Either way, with the right references and/or tactics, Ahluwalia could have obtained that position. And, clearly, he did.
Most of all, I am stunned that Ahluwalia is still working in academia.
Me too. Frankly I’m surprised Imperial accepted him for a PhD. Data fabrication is the worst kind of fraud; I can see a kind supervisor giving someone a second chance if they’d been found guilty of plagiarism, say. But not making up your data.
I wonder if his PhD work at Imperial was all kosher?
See entries on 26th January at:
http://www.retractionwatch.com/2010/11/26/update-on-ahluwalia-fraud-case-researcher-faked-results-probably-committed-sabotage-says-ucl/#more-909
Sherlock
Probably not there has already been one erratum published for the papers published from his PhD thesis and now Imperial is apparently relooking into his data.
According to the erratum the drug concentrations used were 1000 times more than originally staed
or he was indeed innocent and all was arranged to destroy his career.
I adore academics, I would like to keep one as a pet in my garden and feed him bonbons.
In Australia divers always carry a knife so that when a shark appears they can cut their dive buddy with it so that the shark will be sense the blood and attack the buddy in preference. How similar, how strikingly similar to academics when the fin of scientific fraud is spotted.
I am of course joking about the divers – not about the academics.